What We Know About Cooperative Learning at the College Level
David Johnson & Roger Johnson
What do we know about the effectiveness of cooperative learning (CL) with
college students? In 1949 Morton Deutsch published his landmark study
comparing cooperative and competitive learning in a college psychology class at
MIT. In 1952 Haines replicated the study at the University of Michigan. In 1957
Thomas extended their work with a study on cooperation created through a
division of labor among female college students. Over one-third of all studies
comparing cooperative, competitive and individualistic efforts have been
conducted on college students. The amount and consistency of the research on
CL makes it one of the most distinguished of all instructional practices for higher
education.
We have recently completed a meta-analysis of the studies using college
students as subjects (Johnson & Johnson, 1993). In our review we have
separated the studies that use individual measures of learning from studies
that use group measures of productivity. Only studies examining individual
learning will be discussed in this article.
ACHIEVEMENT
Over 120 studies have compared the relative efficacy of cooperative,
competitive, and individualistic learning on individual achievement. While the
first was conducted in 1924, 70% have been conducted since 1970. In these
studies, CL promoted higher individual achievement than did competitive
(effect size=0.54) or individualistic (effect size=0.51) learning. When only the
methodologically high quality studies were included, CL still promoted greater
individual achievement than did competitive or individualistic efforts (effect
sizes of 0.61 and 0.35 respectively). These results held for verbal tasks (such as
reading, writing, and orally presenting), mathematical tasks, and procedural
tasks (such as swimming, golf, and tennis). The research on achievement has
also found cooperation to promote greater intrinsic motivation to learn, more
frequent use of cognitive processes such as reconceptualization, higher-level
reasoning, metacognition, cognitive elaboration, and networking, and greater
long-term maintenance of the skills learned.
OTHER OUTCOMES
There are other important outcomes at the college level besides achievement.
Retaining students until graduation, creating a learning community, and
building positive relationships among diverse students are three examples. In
the 38 studies that included interpersonal attraction as a dependent variable
(including esprit-de-corps, cohesiveness, trust, and other relationship
variables), cooperative efforts promoted greater liking among subjects than did
competing with others (effect size=0.68 or working on one's own (effect size
=0.55). In the 24 studies that focused on social support, furthermore, students
learning cooperatively felt more social support (both academically and
personally) from peers and professors than did students working competitively
(effect size=0.60) or individualistically (effect size=0.51). When students worked
cooperatively, positive and supportive relationships tended to develop, even
among students from different ethnic, cultural, language, social class, ability,
and gender groups.
PSYCHOLOGICAL HEALTH
Several studies conducted in a number of very different college-age
populations, have found cooperativeness highly correlated with a wide variety
of indices of psychological health, and individualistic attitudes related to a wide
variety of indices of psychological pathology. Competitiveness was related to a
complex mixture of indices of health and pathology. One important aspect of
psychological health is self-esteem. The 13 studies that have been conducted
found that cooperation promotes higher self-esteem than did competitive
(effect size=0.47) or individualistic (effect size=0.29) efforts. Members of
cooperative groups also become more socially skilled than do students working
competitively or individualistically.
WHY TAKE THIS RESEARCH SERIOUSLY?
Here are five reasons why college instructors should take this research on CL
seriously:
1. CL has a rich history of theory, research, and practice.
There are at least three general theoretical perspectives that have guided
research on CL. The most influential is social interdependence theory, whose
roots extend from Kurt Koffka in the early 1900s to Kurt Lewin and Morton
Deutsch in the mid- 1900s, to the authors of this article. Its central proposition
is that the way social interdependence is structured determines how
individuals interact with each other which, in turn, determines outcomes.
Positive interdependence (cooperation) creates promotive interaction, negative
interdependence (competition) creates oppositional interaction, and no
interdependence (individualistic efforts) results in an absence of interaction.
From the theories of Piaget and Vigotsky comes the cognitive development
theory with the proposition that when individuals cooperate, socio-cognitive
conflict occurs that creates cognitive disequilibrium, which in turn stimulates
perspective-taking ability and cognitive development. Both the work on
structuring academic controversies and research on cognitive restructuring
are drawn from this orientation. Based on the work of Skinner, Bandura,
Homans, Thibaut and Kelly, and, more recently, Slavin, behavioral learning
theory focuses on the impact of group contingencies on learning; its main
proposition is that actions followed by extrinsic rewards will be repeated.
2. The research on CL has a validity and generalizability rarely found in the
education literature.
A wide variety of tasks, ways of structuring cooperation, and measures of the
dependent variables have been used. Subjects have varied as to age sex,
economic class, nationality, and cultural background. Demonstration studies
have been conducted in real classrooms over extended periods of time. The
research has been conducted by scholars with markedly different orientations,
working in different settings and countries, over a period of nine decades.
3. CL affects many different instructional outcomes simultaneously.
As already noted, these include achievement, positive relationships among
students, and psychological health. CL is, therefore, a very cost-effective
instructional procedure.
4. Quite a bit is known about the essential components that make it work.
Placing students in groups and telling them to work together does not in and of
itself result in cooperation. To structure lessons so students do in fact work
cooperatively requires (a) an understanding of the components that make
cooperation work (positive interdependence, individual accountability,
promotive interaction, social skills, and group processing), and (b) the
disciplined structuring of these components in every learning group and in
every lesson. Instructors must carefully structure positive interdependence to
ensure that all students are committed to each other as persons and to each
other's success. Each student must be held accountable for exerting maximum
efforts to learn by his or her peers. Students must meet and promote each
other's learning face to face. The teamwork skills required to coordinate efforts
to complete joint assignments must be directly taught and mastered. Finally,
team members must gather data on their own progress and plan how to
improve the process they are using to learn. These five essential elements are
not simply characteristics of good CL groups; they are a discipline that must be
implemented rigorously to produce the conditions for effective cooperative
action.
5. Finally, CL creates learning opportunities that do not exist when students
work competitively or individually.
The most obvious is that the use of CL groups promotes the simultaneous
learning of both academic and teamwork skills. CL groups also provide a setting
in which students can (a) construct and extend conceptual understanding of
what is being learned through explanations and discussion; (b) use the shared
mental models learned in flexible ways to solve problems jointly; (c) receive
interpersonal feedback as to how well the procedures are performed; (d)
receive social support and encouragement to take risks in increasing one's
competencies; (e) be held accountable by peers to practice and practice until the
procedures and skills being taught are over learned; (f) acquire attitudes (such
as continuous improvement) needed to refine the procedures learned; (g)
establish a shared identity with other group members; and (h) observe the
most outstanding group members as behavioral models to be emulated. While
these opportunities are not guaranteed, and do not automatically occur in
every CL group, the likelihood of their occurrence in competitive and
individualistic situations is very low.
IN CONCLUSION
For all these reasons, taking this research seriously results in the conclusion
that CL should be used the majority of time in college classes. While CL is not
perfect, and is certainly not a panacea that solves all instructional problems
experienced by college professors, it has been demonstrated to be superior
under most conditions to competitive and individualistic learning.
The use of CL, however, does not mean that no individual work should be
required. Individual practice is usually necessary, and competitive and
individualistic activities can be used to supplement and enhance CL (Johnson,
Johnson, & Holubec, 1992). Since poorly structured CL groups may be
counter-productive, furthermore, professors have to be judicious consumers
who can tell the difference between CL groups in which the essential elements
have been carefully structured and traditional learning groups in which they
have not. Thus, college professors have the difficult responsibility of increasing
he use of well-structured CL groups while decreasing the instructional use of
poorly structured groups.
References
Johnson, D., Johnson, R., & Holubec, E. (1992). Advanced Cooperative Learning.
Edina, MN: Interaction Book Co.
Johnson, D. & Johnson, R. (1993). Cooperative, competitive, and Individualistic
procedures for educating adults: A comparative analysis. University of
Minnesota, Cooperative Learning Center (submitted for publication).
This article was originally published in Cooperative Learning , Vol. 13, No. 3.
Copyright 1993, IASCE. Used with permission.